Introduction
Challenging conventional understandings of centre
Experiencing "now" through questions rather than answers
Cognitive catastrophes and their associated questions?
Configuration of question-pairs
Patterns of questions indicative of the subunderstanding of now
Polyhedral configuration of questions
Musical implications of orbifolds for comprehension of questioning dynamics
Questions undermining integrative insight and initiatives
Implications of question configuration in practice
Mapping of WH-questions with question-pairs onto a memorable polyhedron (a football)
Mapping of WH-questions with question-pairs onto the Szilassi polyhedron
Potential insights into the Szilassi configuration of WH-questions from 4D
Reframing nothing as a vital focus for sustainability
References
Annex to Being neither Dead nor Alive: but how to know now? (2014)
The main paper clarifies understandings of "feeling alive" and "feeling dead" in this strange period , and the associated ambiguities, as a means of framing the strange experience of "now". That argument concluded with sections on:
The latter section drew attention to the existence of unusual geometric forms which could serve metaphorically as "containers" for cognitive engagement with "now" -- and to the challenge of their comprehension. This was seen as a key to enabling and enhancing the sense of "feeling alive".
This argument is developed further here by reframing "centre" as a curious form of "hole" which, like the black holes of astrophysics, functions cognitively as a strange attractor. As such, in the moment, the experience of "now" is a continuing invitation to "go down the rabbit hole" -- as framed for children by Charles Lutwidge Dodgson under the pseudonym Lewis Carroll (Alice's Adventures in Wonderland, 1865). Dodgson was a mathematician and logician (The Game of Logic, 1887).
Entering a "rabbit hole" has been understood as framing a period of chaos or confusion -- appropriate to the current condition of global civilization and the challenge of individual response to it. The phrase has featured as the subtitle of a DVD version of the much-cited film What the Bleep Do We Know!? (2004), What the Bleep! Down the Rabbit Hole - Quantum Edition (2006), following a book version What the Bleep Do We Know!? Discovering the Endless Possibilities of Your Everyday Reality (2005). This combines documentary-style interviews, computer-animated graphics, and a narrative that posits a spiritual connection between quantum physics and consciousness -- framed as the foundation of future thought.
The following images serve as an introductory illustration of some alternative framings of "centre" in relation to "hole". They derive from arguments presented separately (World Introversion through Paracycling: global potential for living sustainably "outside-inside", 2013).
Animation of Sphere-Torus transformation (Reproduced from Wikimedia Commons) |
Animation of Klein bottle (by Konrad Polthier) (Imaging Maths: inside the Klein bottle, +Plus Magazine, September 2003; [click for animated version or explore java applet] |
The implications of the toroidal features of the images above have been developed separately and more specifically in:
The images below -- intertwining two tori -- are suggestive of the implications of "going down" any "cognitive "rabbit hole".
Screen shots of a dynamic virtual
reality model of intertwined tori (click on each variant to access and manipulate in 3D; in the free Cortona VRML viewer, right click for preferences to switch from/to the "wireframe" presentation) |
|||
Red torus has a vortex (smoke ring) dynamic in the model | |||
Blue torus has a wheel-like dynamic in the model | |||
VRML animations by Bob Burkhardt. |
The importance of "now" is a theme explored by several authors (Eckhart Tolle, The Power of Now: a guide to spiritual enlightenment, 1997; Peter Russell, Spirit of Now; Peter Senge, et al, Presence: human purpose and the field of the future, 2008). Potentially more relevant to the sense of "feeling alive" for many, it is the principal theme of many songs -- characterized to a remarkable degree by WH-questions aroused by romantic relationships. The issue here is how focus is given through questions on the nature of "now" in feeling alive. The potential of questions in this respect has also been considered separately in relation to "enlivening" organizations (World Futures Conference as Catastrophic Question: from performance to morphogenesis and transformation, 2013). There is a case for exploring "now" as a question rather than an answer.
Physics frames reality objectively in terms of a complex known as space-time -- combining space and time into a single interwoven continuum. This essentially fails to address the question of how it is possible to feel alive within this context. The argument can however be developed by recognizing that "space" implies the question "where" and "time" the question "when" -- both being questions significant to the immediate sense of being alive. This suggests the merit of recognizing the role of other key questions vital to that experience. Commonly recognized are the so-called WH-questions, of which (as discussed below) there are possibly seven: where, when, what, which, who, why, and how.
Some possibilities are indicated by the following:
Catastrophes? Questions can be compared to "cognitive catastrophes" through the existential disruption with which they are associated. As separately argued (Cognitive Feel for Cognitive Catastrophes: Question Conformality, 2006), a sense of the characteristics of such cognitive feel for discontinuity or disharmony may be obtained from personal recollection of the joy (or agony) occasioned by the following common WH-questions:
The Who and What variants notably reflect the self-reflexive challenge to the objective certainty assumed in the typically external focus of Which and How. The latter readily assume Who and What to be knowns in any decision-making context -- ignoring (existential) uncertainties potentially addressed by Who and What.
Semiophysics: Understood as catastrophes, there is a case for exploring the sense in which they may correspond to the 7 so-called "elementary catastrophes" of catastrophe theory, as elaborated by René Thom (Structural Stability and Morphogenesis, 1972). The possibility was explored separately (Conformality of 7 WH-questions to 7 Elementary Catastrophes: an exploration of potential psychosocial implications, 2006) and gave rise to the following table.
Thom's later work highlighted the possibility of "semiophysics" as a general theory of intelligibility (Semiophysics: a sketch, 1990; Jean Petitot, La Semiophysic: del a physique qualitative aux sciences cognitives, 1994; Peeter Müürsepp, Semiophysics as a Theoretical Basis for Scientific Creativity, 1998; F.T. Arecchi, Complexity and emergence of meaning: toward a semiophysics, 2001)
WH-questions in relation
to the "elementary catastrophes"of catastrophe theory
|
|||||||
Singularities |
Organizing |
Physical examples |
Dynamics
|
Archetypal morphologies |
WH-questions |
||
Destructive |
Constructive |
Question |
Property |
||||
Fold |
V=x3 |
Edge, end; refraction of sunlight by raindrops to form a rainbow | Being |
Ending |
Beginning |
When |
Time |
Cusp |
V=x4 |
Fault; geological fault; transitions from flight to fight, love to hate, and anxiety to calm in man and animals | Becoming
|
Capturing, Separating, Breaking |
Engender, Uniting, Becoming |
Where | Location |
Swallowtail |
V=x5 |
Slit, crack; behavior patterns in some human nervous disorders; structural stability and buckling | Agitate |
Rejecting, Tearing, Splitting |
Crossing, Knitting |
Which (Whether) |
Distinction |
Butterfly |
V=x6 |
Pocket, shell; structural stability and buckling | Give |
Sending, Scaling, Exfoliating |
Receiving, Giving |
How | Dynamic |
Hyperbolic umbilic |
V=x3+y3 |
Arch; collapse of bridges; development of sonar devices | Cresting wave |
Collapsing, Breaking (wave), Breaking down |
Covering |
What | Typology,
Taxonomy Nomenclature |
Elliptic umbilic |
V=x3-3xy2 |
Needle, hair; flow of fluids | Penetrate |
Piercing |
Filling, Annihilating |
Who Whom Whose |
Identity, Nomenklatura, Authenticity |
Parabolic umbilic |
V=x2y+y4 |
Fountain, mushroom, mouth; atmospheric fronts; problems in the field of linguistics; elastic stability | Eject |
Lancing, Pinching |
Linking, Opening |
Why Wherefore |
Reason, Symbolism, Auspiciousness Value Aesthetic |
Variety of questions: Clearly both the number of "WH-questions" and their association with catastrophes could be challenged. The association does however suggest a means of clarifying the distinctions between the questions. The number cited seems to vary in the literature. The Wikipedia entryolley on the Five Ws, adds a single "H", but offers an extensive discussion (with valuable historical references) which could extend or limit the array. It could be argued that the distinctions are subject to the strictures of the much-cited paper of George Miller (The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: some limits on our capacity for processing information, Psychological Review, 1956). Identification of key questions could extend to criminal investigation and intelligence gathering.
Useful indications are offered by the following contrasts:
Arguments favouring 5 Ws (with an H) tend to conflate "what" and "which" from the array of 6 Ws (with an H).
Inappropriate variety? This argument, as with previous discussion, therefore focuses on 7 WH-questions with the potential implication that omission of any one of them may be indicative of a degree of questioning inadequacy in relation to the experience of "now". For example, any tendency to omit "which", notably by journalists, would be indicative of a failure to address a fundamental challenge of decision-makers confronted by a variety of options (elicited and distinguished by "what?").
The argument can be developed through considering how the consequence of intelligence gathering (of relevance to "now") may be variously distorted by omission of one (or more) of the WH-questions in the array. More problematic, what is the consequence of deliberately designing one (or more) questions "off the table", as variously explored separately (Lipoproblems: Developing a Strategy Omitting a Key Problem, 2009; Strategic Implications of 12 Unasked Questions in Response to Disaster, 2013; Systematic Gerrymandering of Declared Threats and Legality of Response, 2013).
The issue can be explored from a cybernetic perspective through the following question: what is the requisite variety of questions to sustain a meaningful sense of "now"? With respect to the viability of any whole. this could be framed as the variety required to avoid some form of "cognitive paraplegia".
The Teaching of Learning Center of the University of Nevada notes that: Research on the questions teachers ask shows that about 60 percent require only recall of facts, 20 percent require students to think, and 20 percent are procedural in nature (Question Types). In providing guidance for interviews, the Student Success Center of the Western Student Services of Canada distinguishes: behaviour-based questions, situational/hypothetical questions, skill-testing questions, problem-solving questions, case based interviews, traditional questions, and illegal questions (Types of Questions). A communications training website asks: Do You Ask a Variety of Questions or the Same Old Ones?. Irene Koshik explores how questioning may be essentially rhetorical, designed to make assertions rather than elicit new information ( WH-questions used as challenges, Discourse Studies).
Of particular interest is the possibility that there are other forms of WH-question considered significant in other languages and cultures -- beyond the set of seven noted above (L. L.-S. Cheng, On the Typology of WH-Questions, 1991; Gabriela Soare, A Cross-linguistic Typology of Question Formation and the Antisymmetry Hypothesis, Generative Grammar in Geneva, 2007). What is the consequence of neglecting them in discourse intended to be of global import? Unfortunately the extensive literature has a formal preoccupation and is only incidentally concerned, if at all, with any unusual experiential relevance of such questions.
Expressed otherwise, how might a much larger array of questioning modalities be "collapsed" into an array of a particular size (such as 7, plus or minus 2) -- or "expanded" to a larger array? What then is the consequence of the loss of the variety inherent in the larger array, or its associated loss of the focus provided by a smaller set? Such questions suggest that any array of WH-questions might be fruitfully and self-reflexively applied to its own elaboration. Can it be said that new varieties of question have been discovered, of a kind implying cognitive processes ignored in English -- questions with strategic implications in the moment?
Answers as assumptions: Another approach might be to consider how the condition of "now" could be framed by unasked questions, namely by answers implied or assumed, rendering questioning unnecessary and inappropriate. This has notably been considered in terms of Max Weber's vision of an "iron cage" of rationalization, and Michel Foucault's carceral archipelago. This too might have cultural implications. These are usefully suggested by languages which have relatively few words to distinguish those colours considered quite distinct otherwise -- through their names in other languages. What kinds of answers are considered unquestionable? Assumed answers -- to who, when, where, what, which, why and how -- then effectively provide a cognitive container rigidly defining aspects of "now". What ("iron") containers go unchallenged by questions, since "now" is then an effectively "frozen" category -- and possibly then of wider concern (Framing the Global Future by Ignoring Alternatives: unfreezing categories as a vital necessity, 2009)?
Multiple WH-questions: Use of space-time as a continuum is a highly significant pairing for fundamental physics, together with the related understanding it offers of world lines. This suggests the value of pairing where-when and the other WH-questions. Literature of some relevance considers "multiple WH-questions" within the same phrase (Catherine Rudin, On Multiple Questions and Multiple WH-fronting, Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 1988; Marina Stoyanova, The Typology of Multiple WH-questions and Language Variation).
As discussed in the main paper, world lines may be understood as having personal experiential implications, suggesting that question-pairs are potentially indicative of their cognitive implications and "feel". Within one's own world, such "lines" might fruitfully be understood as associations and correspondences by which imagination is transported "globally. With any reference to "capturing the imagination", the imagination is then usefully understood to be captured by (complex bundles) of such world lines.
So paired, the the 7 WH-questions give rise to the 42 pairs in the following table. These are reduced to 21, if (for example) when-where and where-when are treated as a single pair. The 21 are then in the portion of the table beneath the diagonal below).
Enabling question-pairs in feeling alive | |||||||
when? | where? | which? | how? | what? | who? | why? | |
when? | -- | ||||||
where? | -- | ||||||
which? | -- | ||||||
how? | -- | ||||||
what? | -- | ||||||
who? | -- | ||||||
why? | -- |
The set of 21 pairs provides a context, perhaps to be understood as a strange form of cognitive container -- a dynamic frame for the sense of feeling alive "now" through the active, moment-by-moment engagement with these questions. As such they offer an immediate challenge to any definitive answer implying closure, as highlighted by the Sanskrit adage Neti Neti -- not this, not that.
The resulting dynamic could be construed as a form of "science" based on questioning rather than one preoccupied with the closure of answering. This is perhaps to be understood as creative ignorance ("nescience"), following the style of apophatic discourse with its particular implications for identity (Being What You Want: problematic kataphatic identity vs. potential of apophatic identity? 2008).
Configurative container: The nature of the container is suggested by the following configuration.
Suggestive configuration of 21 question-pairs enabling feeling alive now (with tentative indication of correspondences to associated cognitive catastrophes from the table above, with the "umbilic" forms tentatively positioned at the top) |
Uncertainty implicit in question pairing: The implication of the Heisenberg Uncertainty Principle have long been accepted with respect to a fundamental property of reality as framed by quantum systems, however this may be confused with arguments relating to observation of that reality.The principle relates to the uncertainty of knowledge of position in space and time -- knowledge of one rendering uncertain (to some degree) any precise knowledge of the other.
Having associated the question-pair where-when with space-time, there is a case for recognizing that the principle applies in some meaure with respect to questions relating to the dimension with which the pair is associated. The argument may then be applied to the other dimensions with which the other question-pairs are associated -- as presented schematically above. Some form of "uncertainty principle", more generally understood, may apply in each case.
The challenge is all the more evident if the question-pairs are understood as unidimensional -- as the lines in the schematic above suggest. The argument is well-made in speculative consideration by mathematicians of entities living on a line in a linear reality -- and their interaction with two- and three-dimensional entities. Whilst this may clarify the where-when case of space-time, the issue here is how analogous insights might apply to the other cases indicated above. Arguably many have direct experience of the uncertainty and ambiguity characteristic of the situations framed above. There is no lack of reference to the challenges of uncertainty at this time (Jonathan Fields, Uncertainty: turning fear and doubt into fuel for brilliance, 2012; Susan Jeffers, Embracing Uncertainty: breakthrough methods for achieving peace of mind when facing the unknown, 2004; M. Granger Morgan, et al, Uncertainty: a guide to dealing with uncertainty in quantitative risk and policy analysis, 1992; Dennis V. Lindley, Understanding Uncertainty, 2006; C. Namwali Serpel, Seven Modes of Uncertainty, 2014). There is clerly a case for confronting the distinctions made in the latter with those highlighted in relation to questions.
The configuration above is of interest in that the relational lines could be used to distinguish a pattern of outlying zones (perhaps to be variously shaded in the image, as shown below). These would be indicative of a focus of partial attention, less intimately related to a more integrative sense of being alive "now". Rollowing the argument of the main paper, they could be explored in terms of degrees of "feeling dead" in that the dynamic associated with particular questions is ignored -- perhaps through dependence on particular answers and the assumption that they should not be "called into question"..
The four animations are based on 3 or 4-fold configurations of distinct pairs of questions from the image above. They might be understood as different ways of avoiding "now", or of forms of the "subunderstanding" of concern to Magoroh Maruyama (Polyocular vision or Subunderstanding? Organization Studies, 2004). The animations offer a sense of different styles of the derivative thinking discussed separately (Vigorous Application of Derivative Thinking to Derivative Problems, 2013). Rather than "derivative", such thinking might be described as "peripheral" to that required to enable and enhance a sense of "now".
Patterns of derivative thinking avoiding "now" as suggested by animations of configurations of pairs of questions |
|||
The elements of the animations could of course be "woven" together into a single more complex animation, phasing the dynamics in a variety of suggestive ways, as argued more generally (Interweaving Thematic Threads and Learning Pathways, 2010).
If only for mnemonic purposes, there is a case for exploring the 21 pairs of questions as corresponding in some way to "cognitive vitamins" -- given arguments for correspondences between the pattern logic of the I Ching, the genetic code, and the amino acid vitamins essential to human life (Martin Schonberger, I Ching and the Genetic Code, 1992), as discussed with respect to Enhancing the Quality of Knowing through Integration of East-West metaphors (2000). The configurations above could then be understood as "cognitive vitamin complexes", thereby associating "cognitive vitamin deficiency" with Maruyama's subunderstanding.
The 2-dimensional configuration above naturally raises the question as to how the pattern might be configured in a more integrative manner in a 3-dimensional representation. For it to be "more integrative", the relations between the question-pairs need to be held in ways which offer memorable insight into mutually reinforcing links -- readily lost in 2 dimensions. The concern would then be whether the 3-dimensional configuration constitutes a more appropriate container for the sense of "now" in the process of "feeling alive". The emphasis is on the identification of more appropriately facilitative mnemonic aids, as separately discussed (In Quest of Mnemonic Catalysts -- for comprehension of complex psychosocial dynamics, 2007).
There is a certain irony to the arguments in the main paper regarding emergence of a cognitive "container culture" -- potentially well-framed by Max Weber's above-mentioned "iron cage" of rationalization -- given the typically polyhedral rectangular form of conventional containers and the global dependence on them. In terms of this argument, are their 6 sides, 12 edges and 8 vertices indicative of an overriding preference for a particular mode of organization and mapping -- with unexamined cognitive implications. The issue here might be framed in terms of how to design more appropriate containers with more fruitful cognitive implications. Also of relevance is the question as to why this is not considered?
As is evident by inspection, the configuration above can be understood as embedded in what is known as the Heawood graph (portrayed below). This is an undirected graph with 14 vertices (of 7 types) and 21 edges (of 12 types). It is a toroidal graph, namely it can be embedded without crossings onto a torus. As shown in the animation below, one embedding of this type places its vertices and edges into three-dimensional Euclidean space as the set of vertices and edges of a nonconvex polyhedron with the topology of a torus: the Szilassi polyhedron (one of the Stewart toroids). [Thanks are due to Heiner Benking for triggering research into this possibility and its relation to previous discussion of orbifolds (see below)]
Rather than associating each WH-question with a vertex (as in the 2-dimensional configuration above), each of the 7 WH-questions could then be associated with one of the 7 hexagonal faces -- of which there are 4 types (all of irregular shape). The tetrahedron and the Szilassi polyhedron are the only two known polyhedra in which each face shares an edge with each other face -- of significance to graph colouring and map colouring. Its relatively comprehensible complexity, and the toroidal form, then offer a richer and more integrative framing of "now" in the light of the above argument. The dual to the Szilassi polyhedron is the Császár polyhedron, which has no diagonals; every pair of vertices is connected by an edge.
Although seemingly obscure, the Szilassi polyhedron figures as one of the works of monumental art -- with the Mobius Strip -- in Reconciliation Place (Canberra). This is located between the National Library and High Court of Australia, as a tribute to the Indigenous people of Australia.
Heawood graph (reproduced from Wikipedia) |
Rotation of Szilassi polyhedron (reproduced from Wikipedia) |
A readily accessible interactive version of the Szilassi polyhedron is available (Lajos Szilassi and Sándor Kabai, The Parametrized Szilassi Polyhedron, Wolfram Demonstrations Project, 2008).
The Heawood graph is a basic feature in discussion of the subtleties of orbifolds (for "orbit-manifold") in the mathematical disciplines of topology, geometry, and geometric group theory. An orbifold is a generalization of a manifold. It is a topological space (an "underlying space") with an orbifold structure. Seemingly incomprehensible to most, orbifolds have been applied to music theory, notably by Dmitri Tymoczko (The Geometry of Musical Chords, Science, 2006; A Geometry of Music, 2011). Given the above argument, the abstract is suggestive of the possibility that the distinctive cognitive feel for the WH question-pairs might be associated with chords:
A musical chord can be represented as a point in a geometrical space called an orbifold. Line segments represent mappings from the notes of one chord to those of another. Composers in a wide range of styles have exploited the non-Euclidean geometry of these spaces, typically by using short line segments between structurally similar chords. Such line segments exist only when chords are nearly symmetrical under translation, reflection, or permutation. Paradigmatically consonant and dissonant chords possess different near-symmetries and suggest different musical uses.
Engagement with music in the moment might be understood as the "art of now" -- a theme otherwise variously discussed (Jay Dixit, The Art of Now: six steps to living in the moment, Psychology Today, 1 November 2008). Music activates integrative associations and correspondences in the moment across the variety of modes of cognition. In framing "now", the "dimension" associated with any question-pair as a polarity might then be explored in terms of basic musical theory (Polarities as Pluckable Tensed Strings: hypercomprehension through harmonics of value-based choice-making, 2006).
A related study of potential relevance is that of Guerino Mazzola (The Topos of Music: geometric logic of concepts, theory, and performance, 2002). In eliciting a sense of "now", of particular concern is whether and how the "music" is capable of sustaining interest. The question could be considered a musical metaphor of the challenge of sustaining the sustainability which is the quest of global governance -- especially its cognitive sustainability. This is significant given the risk of its obsolescence, meaning how innovative concepts undergo a career of stages or phases, a life-cycle in other words -- as noted by Johan Galtung with respect to "concept careers" within the UN system (Processes in the UN system, 1980). Ironically the issue also relates to traditional implications of heavenly music and its eternal nature -- the music of immortality?
Given the widespread popular appeal of music to all sectors of society, the vulnerability of the current global civilization to catastrophic (over)simplification (discussed in the main paper), merits discussion in musical terms. Music and poetry offer means of encompassing complexity, as argued by Gregory Bateson in explaining why "we are our own metaphor" to a conference on the effects of conscious purpose on human adaptation:
One reason why poetry is important for finding out about the world is because in poetry a set of relationships get mapped onto a level of diversity in us that we don't ordinarily have access to. We bring it out in poetry. We can give to each other in poetry the access to a set of relationships in the other person and in the world that we're not usually conscious of in ourselves. So we need poetry as knowledge about the world and about ourselves, because of this mapping from complexity to complexity. (Mary Catherine Bateson. Our Own Metaphor, 1972, pp. 288-289)
Some possibilities have been discussed separately:
However both music and poetry also lend themselves to over-simplification inappropriate to the cognitive challenges of the time. Hyothetically, what factors ensure that "heavenly music" -- the music of the spheres -- does not become boring in an eternal context?
With respect to a poetic approach to the set of questions, they have been famously memorialized for children by Rudyard Kipling in the following poem -- by implication in relation to combat in the Afghanistan arena. The decision-making emphasis of "Which" is notably absent.
I Keep Six Honest... by Rudyard Kipling from the The Elephant's Child, one of the Just So Stories (1902) |
|
I keep six honest serving-men (They taught me all I knew); Their names are What and Why and When And How and Where and Who. I send them over land and sea, I send them east and west; But after they have worked for me, I give them all a rest. |
I let them rest from nine till five, For I am busy then, As well as breakfast, lunch, and tea, For they are hungry men. But different folk have different views. I know a person small- She keeps ten million serving-men, Who get no rest at all! |
She sends'em abroad on her own affairs, From the second she opens her eyes- One million Hows, two million Wheres, And seven million Whys! |
Kipling's poem, more specifically regarding Afghanistan (The Young British Soldier, 1895), has recently been rewritten in the light of experience there (Ryan Kisiel, "When you're lying alone in your Afghan bivvy...": British soldier re-writes Rudyard Kipling poem in damning attack on conditions, Mail Online, 4 August 2009).
The seemingly obscure indication of catastrophes acquires considerable significance (as with the associated questions) in considering the preoccupations of those who most obviously exemplify a sense of being alive "now" -- namely those engaged in the visibly complex manoeuvers of extreme acrobatic sports (snowboarding, skateboarding, skiing, freestyle BMX, and inline skating).
The argument could be extended to certain martial arts and to the skills of improvisation (Multivocal Poetic Discourse Emphasizing Improvisation: clarification of possibilities for the future, 2012).
The sense of "now" in being alive is necessarily associated with an integrative cognitive dynamic, as is evident in the case of those sports and skills, and in the appreciation of music. In an earlier exercise a set of question-pairs was used to focus on the manner whereby any proposed integrative framework tends to be undermined -- effectively to collapse the associated potential.
Questions of confidence to which deficient integrity is vulnerable (reproduced from In-forming the Chalice as an Integrative Cognitive Dynamic: sustaining the Holy Grail of global governance, 2011) |
||||
. | where? | when? | which? | why? |
who? | accredited where? "unrecognized" institution? |
accredited when? too new? too old? |
which school of thought? a deprecated perspective? |
usual suspects? other agendas? |
how? | impractical there? facilities? know-how? |
premature? too little, too late? |
how to decide? how to reconcile alternatives? |
why that way? how to know why? |
what? | appropriate there? where to do what? |
appropriate then? when to do what? |
which is appropriate? what is best? |
why that? what for? |
These may be considered variants of the fundamental concern at this time: why should we have confidence, in what, and as proposed by whom? This suggests that the configuration above can be explored as a container of confidence with which feeling alive is intimately associated. (Primary Global Reserve Currency: the Con? Cognitive implications of a prefix for sustainable confidelity, 2011)
Framed by the above argument:
And if the Wine you drink, the Lip you press, |
In the quest for ways of mapping WH-questions, and their pairing, one approach -- illustrated below -- is to configure 6 questions as hexagons. The contiguous edges then allow for mapping the question-pairs. However, the hexagonal metaphor privileges a single question -- the seventh at the centre. This suggests that this mapping is only useful if each question can take up the central position -- namely a dynamic mapping, rather than a static one. This could be valuable as a framing for the "now" which it is argued that the pattern of questions provides..
Indication of hexagonal array of WH-questions -- implying a resonance dynamic with other variants |
The possible dynamic would then be better understood by configuration in three dimensions as a polyhedron. One widely recognizable form, which lends itself to the hexagonal patterning, is the truncated icosahedron -- most commonly recognized in the pattern of stitching of a football. It has 20 hexagonal faces and 12 pentagonal faces. Attribution of 20 questions to the hexagonal faces recalls the spoken parlor game Twenty Questions, which encourages deductive reasoning and creativity.
In that case however, the different hexagonal patterns (each with a central WH-question) is separated from the others by pentagonal shape. The result is illustrated in the image and animation below. A preliminary assumption is made that each WH-question can figure twice at the centre of a hexagonal pattern -- hence the indication of "a" and "b" variants. The two variants may well distinguish orderings of the hexagons around of different significance around each pentagon -- within the pattern as a whole.
Preliminary exercise in mapping WH-questions onto faces of a truncated icosahedron | |
2-dimensional unfolded net presentation | Animation of polyhedron |
Mirror pairing: As a more appropriate mapping surface, the Szilassi polyhedron (presented earlier as an animation) is indicative of its challenge to comprehension. However the symmetry of that polyhedron, facilitating its comprehension, is evident from the image below through the manner in which the shapes of 6 faces are paired as mirror images. Only one of the 7 faces is unpaired. As a preliminary exercise, each of the WH-questions is arbitrarily mapped onto a face. The following images and animations have been prepared with the aid of the remarkable Stella: Polyhedron Navigator software, produced by Robert Webb (Stella: Polyhedron Navigator, Culture and Science, 2000).
Face shapes of Szilassi polyhedron presented to show pattern of mirroring with tentative attribution of questions to faces |
Selected views of an indicative mapping of question-pairs onto edges of Szilassi polyhedron (variously rotated; pairs of edge types are identically coloured) |
|
"How" and "Which" faces (in red above) rendered transparent | Only central face rendered visible |
Nets and folding: The Szilassi polyhedron, when unfolded into 2-dimensions, can be understood as being composed of two distinct nets, as presented below. These offer a sense of how the 6-sides faces bind together when folded -- each face being in contact with each of the 6 other faces. Each edge is then a question-pair (as discussed above). These correspond to the contiguous faces. This is clearer in the case of each net separately. It is however vital to emphasize that, like the space-time of fundamental physics, each edge is a continuum. This may be more meaningful in experience in the moment than it is through any formal description. Assciation of the 3 umbilic catastrophes to the smaller net merits future reflection in the light of semiophysics.
Face shapes of Szilassi polyhedron (from above) presented in their 2 distinct nets with the tentative attribution of questions to faces -- and consequent indication of some edge pairing when folded (first and second net on different scales; paired shapes coloured distinctively) |
A sense of how the two nets are folded separately into three dimensions is given in the following two sets of images. It is appropriate to note that the software by which these forms have been explored permits the 2-dimensional nets to be output on paper with tabs, permitting them to be folded and glued into 3-dimensional models. Paired shapes are coloured the same (below), in contrast to the distinct colouring of all shapes in the net presentation (above).
Stages in folding first net of the Szilassi polyhedron (images at various scales to facilitate representation; ; paired shapes coloured identically) |
|||
Flat (as above) | Slightly folded 1 | More folded | Completely folded |
Stages in folding second net of the Szilassi polyhedron (images at various scales; ; paired shapes coloured identically) |
|||
Flat (as above) | Slightly folded | More folded | Completely folded |
It is potentially significant that comprehension is facilitated through manipulation of the 3-dimensional form -- in contrast to the expectations and assumptions of what might be termed a "flat Earth" mentality dependent on a 2-dimensional representation. The point has been specifically argued with respect to a more complex polyhedron (Carlo H. Séquin and Jaron Lanier, Hyperseeing the Regular Hendecachoron). The need for "hypercomprehension" can be explored more generally (Hyperaction through Hypercomprehension and Hyperdrive, 2006).
Animations of Szilassi polyhedron (paired shapes coloured identically) |
|
Folding together of both nets indicated above (some phases illustrated in screen shots above) |
Rotation of polyhedron (variation on version presented above) |
Detailed views through the toroidal "hole" of the Szilassi polyhedron | |
View from one side | View from second side |
As being potentially indicative of the dynamic experience of "now":
Of potential significance is the meaning of (in)visibility in this context, especially if it is suggestive of question-pairs whose nature is ignored or characterized by a unquestioning assumption. Being especially characteristic of (executive) decision, Where-When and Which-How (although notably orthogonal) may be deliberately or inadvertently dissociated from those potentially subtler questioning processes directly associated with the central hole.
The pattern of question-pairs is fruitfully suggestive of a system of cognitive decision processes whose integrative nature is lost when mapped into the 2 dimensions of any systems diagram or semantic map. It can be understood as indicative of the flow of attention associated with the sense of "now" -- and sustaining it.
Bounding spheres: The following images display the Szilassi polyhedron with circumsphere and midsphere.
Szilassi polyhedron with circumsphere | |
Spheres with faces visible | Spheres with faces transparent |
Of potential relevance is the sense in which the outer sphere relates to externalities (objective knowns) typical of conventional executive decision-making, whereas the inner sphere relates to questions otherwise held to be implicit or abstract (mysteriously subjective and paradoxically self-reflexive) -- a contrast between explication and implication.
Types of edges as question-pairs: The Szilassi polyhedron is also of interest as a mapping surface because it offers insights into the relationship between the 12 types of its 21 edges. This can be more readily understood in two dimensions through the following schematic. This treats the 12 types as axes through the centre of a circle -- a focus for the sense of "now". The edges common to the two distinct nets are used as the primary orthogonal axes -- marked with an asterisk. The face shapes are then positioned at the 24 positions on the circumference -- with the contiguous edges on that axis. There are two separate variants of each in the polyhedron -- implying an alternation between the two in the image (with the exception of those markeed with an asterisk). The 24 circumferential positions are therefore reduced by 3 due to those marked with the asterisk.
Presentation of the 12 types of the 21 edges of the Szilassi polyhedron (each of the 12 axes is an edge type; two edge variants for those without asterisk, according to label; hexagonal face shapes are to scale; paired shapes coloured identically) |
The distribution of shapes/axes could no doubt be improved to render the pattern more memorable -- especially through an animated variant. The criteria for an improved distribution include: significant positioning of the 3 axes marked with an asterisk; special (related treatment) of unpaired (green) element; handling of colouring of paired face types and their distinction as individual types; achieving memorable patterning of colours/shapes within quadrants (or octants), maximizing variety and minimizing replication of colour or shape within them; exploitation of animation to hold more complex significance (and render the display visually attractive). Given the seeming simplicity (only 4 face types), the challenge is somewhat reminiscent of the arrangement of codons in the genetic code, or of the 8-fold arrangement of trigrams in the Chinese BaGua system -- suggestive of the possibility of a memetic code (M. Pitkänen, Could one find a geometric realization for genetic and memetic codes? 2013).
Experimental alternation to clarify relation between question-pairs (preliminary exercise) (faces in pairs are coloured distinctively in contrast to the single colour per face type above) |
|
Configuration A | Configuration B |
Alternation between Configuration A and B |
The focus on 12 can be associated with the mapping above onto the truncated icosahedron with its 12 pentagonal faces -- providing centers for a configuration totalling 20 hexagonal faces. However the focus on 12 is especially important given the strategic preference for 12-fold articulations and the systemic possibility of integrative approaches (Checklist of 12-fold Principles, Plans, Symbols and Concepts: web resources, 2011; Eliciting a 12-fold Pattern of Generic Operational Insights: recognition of memory constraints on collective strategic comprehension, 2011; Implication of the 12 Knights in any Strategic Round Table, 2014). Potentially valuable to this possibility is the clarification of the relation of WH-question pairs to preferences for a 12-fold pattern.
Further mapping possibilities: Various indications can be taken into account in elaborating a more memorable pattern -- potentially with mathematical assistance. Of interest is whether each quadrant offers a distinctive "story" by suitably sequencing the items there according to possibilities such as: edge length on axis, area of face, variation of faces between first and second net, positioning of unpaired face. There are various design possibilities for the animation: timing, changing direction, of forms, and colouring. As noted above, although matching edges to shapes derives directly from the properties of the polyhedron, attribution of "questions" to faces is somewhat arbitrary (despite the suggestions from catastrophe theory).
Use of the cuboctahedron as a mapping surface would seem to offer a number of complementary possibilities. As with the Császár polyhedron (the dual of the Szilassi polyhedron), it has 14 faces corresponding to the 14 vertex types of the Szilassi polyhedron. It is however a quasiregular polyhedron, with more symmetry properties, potentially an aid to memory with appropriate colouring. The 24 edges correspond to the mapping of Szilassi edges to the circle (prior to the special the reduction by 3). The cuboctahedron is central to the reflections of R. Buckminster Fuller (Synergetics: explorations in the geometry of thinking, 1975/1979), as discussed separately (Geometry of Thinking for Sustainable Global Governance: cognitive implication of synergetics, 2009).
Related insights could be derived from the work on management cybernetics of Stafford Beer (Beyond Dispute: the invention of team syntegrity, 1994; Platform for Change, 1978). This combined the mapping applications of the icosahedron with the development of a viable system model (Gunter Nittbaur, Stafford Beer's Syntegration as a Renascence of the Ancient Greek Agora in Present-day Organizations, Journal of Universal Knowledge Management, 2005). This raises the question as to whether a fruitful experience of "now" could be understood as enabled by a "viable cognitive system" of which the dynamic configuration of questions offers an indication.
This cybernetic approach has been notably developed by Maurice Yolles (Knowledge Cybernetics: a new metaphor for social collectives 2005; Exploring Mindset Agency Theory, 2013), taking account of both Chinese frameworks and the mindscape perspective of Magoroh Maruyama. There is a case for associating the distinctive "cognitive feel" of WH-questions -- framing "now" -- with the distinctive sense of different mindscapes (Mindscapes, social patterns and future development of scientific theory types, Cybernetica, 1980, 23, 1, pp. 5-25; Context and Complexity: cultivating contextual understanding, 1992). It is in this light that Maruyama could be understood as arguing for "polyocular vision" (Polyocular vision or subunderstanding? Organization Studies, 2004). Such arguments are indicative of the possibility of a "cybernetics of now", potentially consistent with various philosophies.
Indication of the association of one WH-question with another (based on data on the Szilassi polyhedron from the Stella Polyhedron Navigator) |
|
Indicates the association between a given WH-question and other WH-questions, as indicated by edge-length, as a percentage of total edge-length for the shape to which the WH-question is mapped. (NB: Self-contiguity is treated as 0%) |
Indication of the explicit and implicit "weight" of WH-questions in discourse | |
Edge-lengths and Areas for a given face, to which a WH-question has been mapped, are presented as a percentage of the total edge-lengths or areas for the polyhedron. On the assumption that the polyhedral configuration is an indication of a viable cognitive system, Edge-lengths and Areas are also presented in terms of the percentage by which they are greater or smaller than an equal contribution (namely one seventh of the total edge-length or total surface area of the polyhedron. |
Current research on word association makes use of MRI brain scanning (Morgan Kelly, Word association: Princeton study matches brain scans with complex thought, News at Princeton, 31 August 2011). There is a case for applying such methods to clarify the nature of the "cognitive feel" associated with use of WH-questions, and issues relating to under-use or over-use of particular sets of of such questions. Of particular interest is any relation to the sense of "now", especially in comparison with uses of corresponding words in other languages (maintenant, ahora, jetzt, nunc, etc). Current work on the conversion of brain waves to music is expected to give rise to an application that can be run on a mobile device for personal experimentation within a year, as has already been done with biofeedback (Kat Austen, We turn brainwaves into sound for music and medicine, New Scientist, 12 April 2014).
Potential insights for operation of the "winged self": As developer of the Bell helicopter, Arthur M. Young was concerned with the decision-making and information processes in piloting one -- as they might apply to the development of a "psychopter". This was envisaged as the "winged self", requiring an analogue to "seat of the pants" skills in the moment (Geometry of Meaning, 1976), as discussed separately (Engendering a Psychopter through Biomimicry and Technomimicry: insights from the process of helicopter development, 2011). His analysis is potentially of relevance to reconciling the cognitive implications of catastrophe theory (as semiophysics) to any discussion of a 12-fold pattern of question-pair types. Its particular importance lies in the "cognitive feel" the pilot of the helicopter must necessarily have for the knowledge management in the moment.
The relevance can be explored through an argument presented separately in a section on Geometry of meaning: an alchemical Rosetta Stone? in the conclusion to a more general discussion (Eliciting a Universe of Meaning -- within a global information society of fragmenting knowledge and relationships, 2013). This included the following table.
12 "measure formulae" distinguished and clustered by Arthur Young (reproduced from The Geometry of Meaning, p. 102) |
||
Actions | States | Relationships |
Position -- L | Moment -- ML | Power -- ML2/T3 |
Velocity -- L/T | Momentum -- ML/T | Inertia -- ML2 |
Acceleration -- L/T 2 | Force -- ML/T 2 | Action -- ML2/T |
Control -- L/T3 | Mass control -- ML/T3 | Work -- ML2/T2 |
NB: Young indicates with respect to this table: The last column is displaced one place.., in order to have the three members on each line 120 degrees apart (p. 102) ... in his circular configuration presented below |
As indicated there, Young comments extensively on the significance of each of these 12 in terms of the cognitive processes of learning/action cycles. Of relevance to the above argument is the co-presence of "states" and "actions" -- the latter implying the process dimension otherwise lacking in a "state-focus". Recognition of a "relationship" dimension suggests a valuable means of transcending that duality.
With the reservations indicated there regarding the tricky cognitive nature inherent in the creativity of the alchemical process, it was argued that there is a case for reviewing the 12 phases of the process as they have been traditionally associated with the signs of the zodiac (although other patterns of phases are also identified). The names for these alchemical processes (as indicated by Wikipedia) have been added to the circular representation in the schematic below. This combines the triangular and square patterns of connectivity in the schematics articulated by Young in relation to the zodiacal pattern -- valuable to many for for mnemonic purposes, if not otherwise.
Zodiacal encoding of 12 "measure formulae" with associated alchemical processes |
These distinctions have been interpreted separately for a variety of psychosocial contexts (Typology of 12 complementary strategies essential to sustainable development,1998; Characteristics of phases in 12-phase learning / action cycles, 1995; Typology of 12 complementary dialogue modes essential to sustainable dialogue, 1998).
As indicative of the kinds of information of relevance to the control of a helicopter, the 12 types of question-pairs could then be very tentatively explored in relation to Young's configuration -- thereby framing the nature of their relevance to the sense of "now" potentially required for the imaginative control of the "winged self". An obvious approach would be to use the distinction between the 3 "inner" faces and the 4 "outer" faces of the polyhedron (above) to frame a table corresponding to Young's 3x4 table (above), Given the subtle nature of the "cognitive feel" involved, a potentially more appropriate approach could be to use the 7 types of vertices of the Szilassi polyhedron -- of which there are 14. With the current mapping these are as follows.
7 Pairs of vertices in Szilassi polyhedron with indication of question-pairs (coloured by vertex pair, as with 12 edge types; edge lengths not to scale in this rotated perspective of the polyhedron, with faces transparent) |
7 Pairs of vertices in image above (distinguished by associated mapping of question-pairs onto the Szilassi polyhedron) |
|
What-Where / What-When / Where-When | Where-When / Where-Who / When-Who |
Which-Where / Which-How / Where-How | Which-How / When-How / Which-When |
How-Who / How-What / Who-What | Who-What / What-Which / Which-Who |
Which-Why / Which-Where / Where-Why | When-How / How-Why / Why-When |
When-Who / When-Why / Who-Why | What-Where / Where-Why / Why-What |
How-Why / How-What / Why-What | Which-Why / Which-Who / Why-Who |
Where-How / Where-Who / How-Who | Which-When / What-Which / What-When |
It is the characteristic of the qualitative nexus, shared within a pair, which could then (in future) be used to dimension the 3x4 table of question pairs as suggested by the pattern below (and as explored in the previous typologies relating to learning/action cycles). For example, Young himself offers various indications (p. 154) including a fourfold distinction between spontaneous act, reaction, observation, and control.
Potential reconciliation of Young's pattern of cognitive functions with that based on questions (uncompleted) | ||||||
Vertex type D | Vertex type E | Vertex type F | Vertex type G | |||
T0 | T1 | T2 | T3 | |||
Vertex type A | Actions |
Who-What | Which-When Where-How |
Where-Who What-When |
Which-Where When-How |
M0L |
Vertex type B | States |
Which-How | When-Who What-Where |
How-What Which-Who |
How-Who What-Which |
ML |
Vertex type C | Relationships |
Where-When | How-Why Which-Why |
Why-What Why-Which |
Why-Where Why-When |
ML2 |
Given Young's helicopter inspiration, there is a certain charm to the resemblance of the Szilassi polyhedron to the twisted rotor blades of a helicopter rotor through which lift and control are ensured. This reinforces implications as to the possibility of a "winged self" -- centered on "now".
There is the possibility that the seeming complexity of the Szilassi configuration could be comprehended otherwise from a 4-dimensional perspective. The Stella software package specifically permits such exploration. As noted by its developer Robert Webb (Stella 4D Manual, 2014) with respect to The Fourth Dimension?: Although we can't experience a four-dimensional object directly in its original form, there are several ways we can get a glimpse of them in 3D. The software supports the following methods: 4D Projection, Cross-Sections, 4D Nets, Cells, and Vertex Figures.
The issue here is whether such an interactive facility could trigger imaginative exploration of forms suggestive of integrative insights -- in this case into the cognitive configuration of the questioning environment framing the experience of "now". The possibility builds on the sense in which the space-time continuum is understood to be necessarily 4-dimensional.
In any simulation of 4 dimensions the 21 question-pair edges are transformed from lines into 21 planes. The questions associated with the 7 faces of the configuration are transformed into as many volumes -- of which one encompasses the 6 others (as represented in 3 dimensions, below).
The argument here is that the 21 question-pairs -- each understood as a continuum -- suggest an even greater degree of multidimensionality. The number of dimensions is of similar order to that argued by physics as being of relevance to the integrative potential of string theory whether the 11-dimensional form called M-theory or the 26 spacetime dimensions for the bosonic string. It remains to be comprehended what "dimensions" might mean in a psycho-social context. The issue has been discussed separately in relation to the concept of brane in string theory (Global Brane Comprehension Enabling a Higher Dimensional Big Tent? Strategic implication in encompassing nothing and coming to naught, 2011).
Experimental projection of Szilassi 3D question configuration into 4D (using Stella software options to create a duoprism based on a 6-sided polygon) |
|
Animation of rotation in 3D | Aimation of simulated rotation in 4D |
With respect to the space-time continuum, the imagination of many has of course been triggered by spaceships -- with some possibility of travel through time. There is some speculation regarding the nature and implication of "timeship" in contrast to "spaceship" (Timeship: Conception, Technology, Design, Embodiment and Operation, 2003; Embodying a Timeship vs. Empowering a Spaceship, 2003). With respect to the many more dimensions cognitively associated with the questioning dynamic (as implied here), it might be asked whether there is a case for focusing on "questships" -- reminiscent of travel framed as a "vision quest". Potentially more suggestive might be "imagination-ship", "image-ship" or "imaginal-ship", as considered separately with respect to the "magic carpets" required for "noonautics" (Magic Carpets as Psychoactive System Diagrams, 2010).
Could such travel be understood metaphorically as a form of "sailing through life" in which the configuration of questions serves as a form of cognitive vessel -- with the "dimensions" indicated by the question-pairs constituting the "rigging" through which navigation is enabled? Curiously there is a tendency to decorate such rigging with lights and flags (bunting), as with the use of strings of Christmas fairy lights or of prayer flags in Tibetan sacred places -- perhaps a faint reminder of such intangible dimensions. There is a certain charm to the circumferential pattern of faces of the Szilassi polyedron -- arrayed above -- which bear a resemblance to a string of prayer flags -- especially if animated.
Despite the lack of any pattern of "rigging", could some think tanks then be fruitfully understood as "questships", as might be separately inferred (Meta-challenges of the Future for Networking through Think-tanks, 2005)?
Such arguments highlight the paradoxical relation between the simplicity, supposedly associated with appreciation of "now", and the complexity it may imply, whether experientially or according to theoretical insights. Intuition may offer a degree of comprehension of such complexity, but without the capacity to articulate and explicate it. Geometrical forms offer a means of navigating between various degrees of complexity, much as with the zooming facilities in interactive exploration of maps. The issue is the degree of simplicity/complexity appropriate to different circumstances.
With respect to the adequacy of any representation of "now", a particular concern is whether it offers an alienating, sterile insight -- one that is "boring" rather than essentially attractive. A related concern is the counter-productive tendency to "grasp" any such insight possessively (Paradoxes of Engaging with the Ultimate in any Guise: living life penultimately, 2012; Beyond Harassment of Reality and Grasping Future Possibilities, 1996).
It is from such perspectives that the Szilassi mapping can be considered a questioning complex -- an appropriately enigmatic challenge to conventional comprehension, especially through enabling interaction with it in 4D.
With respect to achieving greater comprehension of the dynamics of that complex of higher dimensionality, the following images are suggestive.
Images suggestive of variously engaging with a higher dimensional questioning dynamic In each case the "lens" is that of the single questioning process encompassing the others "Where-ing" and "When-ing" relate notably to space and time respectively |
|
"Where-ing" | "When-ing" |
"How-ing" | "What-ing" | "Which-ing" | "Who-ing" | "Why-ing" |
The arrows in the images are indicative of a form of directionality to the dynamic -- with the suggestion that in each case the encompassing dynamic "meshes" with those encompassed, although the directionality between them "clashes". The higher dimensional condition in which such clashing is resolved is associated with the 12-fold typing of the 21 edges with which the question pairs are associated (as discussed above). It is here that mapping of those hexagonal circular arrangements onto a dodecahedron of 12 faces is helpful -- with the suggestion that there is a dynamic alternation in the directionality. This approach was developed separately (Flowering of Civilization -- Deflowering of Culture: flow as a necessarily complex experiential dynamic, 2014) in a section on Arranging the flowers to engender an ecosystem?. This representation offers a sense of "now" as accessible through 12 "rabbit holes" of more comprehensible nature, rather than through a singular one more challenging to comprehension.
Alternative animations of a "12-flower" dodecahedron -- accessing the "rabbit hole" (an "untruncated" football") |
|
Nothing: The argument has highlighted the association between the sense of "now" and the nature of "nothing" through suggesting that there is particular value in understanding any experiential focus in terms of a central "hole" -- implying a virtual centre rather than a central "point". The topological form most appropriate to this understanding is the torus -- in its most general terms. The toroidal Szilassi polyhedron is an exceptional form for the exploration of this possibility.
Mapping questions and question-pairs onto a toroidal form then "reframes nothing" in a way which is potentially more meaningful experientially -- especially in the light of the attention activated by a dynamic complex of questions. The potential significance of a hole has been succinctly highlighted in a classic Chinese quotation -- with which Stafford Beer's 30-fold icosahedral focus is consistent.
Value of nothing |
Thirty spokes share the wheel's hub; It is the centre hole that makes it useful. Shape clay into a vessel; It is the space within that makes it useful. Cut doors and windows for a room; It is the holes which make it useful. Therefore profit comes from what is there; Usefulness from what is not there. (Lao Tzu, Tao Te Ching). |
Framing nothing using more complex topological forms, by which experiential significance can be articulated, offers further possibilities -- of which the above quotation is indicative. The approach contrasts with the focus on changing from "something" to another "thing" without exploring the possibility that the role of "nothing" may offer richer possibilities. This applies to arguments for a paradigm shift and in the advocacy of alternatives as caricatured separately (Responsibility for Global Governance Who? Where? When? How? Why? Which? What? 2008). Centering on nothing may prove more viable -- and more consistent with a sense of "now" and immediacy, namely with life in the moment.
Of particular relevance is the relatively unexplored nature of holes and the attraction they exert -- hence the reference in the subtitle to going down the "rabbit hole", notably referring to the documentary (What the Bleep! Down the Rabbit Hole). In the latter case, the nature of the associated "nothingness" becomes more mysterious, as remarkably discussed by Roberto Casati and Achille C. Varzi (Holes and Other Superficialities, 1994) -- with respect to the borderlines of metaphysics, everyday geometry, and the theory of perception (as they summarize in the entry on holes in the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy).
Expressed otherwise, it is not that global civilization needs a "hole in the head", but rather that a "hole in the mind" may elicit new forms of insight. Hence the case for a "whole in the globe". Humanity may be in a condition characteristic of some animals, namely under pressure to change skin or carapace -- in cognitive terms. Again the football is suggestive in that respect. It "works" because of "what is not there" -- in the terms of the Tao Te Ching. Of related relevance may be the arguments of Terrence W. Deacon (What's Missing from Theories of Information? 2010) and of Abraham Flexner (The Usefulness of Useless Knowledge, Harper's Magazine, 1939).
If the noosphere is to be understood as being encompassed by some analogue to the ionosphere -- so vital to global telecommunication and remote sensing -- it is then curiously appropriate to note that the use of so-called loop antenna for such purposes could be understood to be of toroidal form. Given the role of circlets of beads in many cultures, there is a certain charm to their comparison with the functions of such an antenna (Designing Cultural Rosaries and Meaning Malas to Sustain Associations within the Pattern that Connects, 2000). As a mapping of decision processes characteristic of the moment, a set of Szilassi polyhedra could be readily strung together as a memento of any so-called "string of decisions" -- or possibly of the challenge of any "vicious cycle" of decisions.
Animation of implied "necklace" of Szilassi polyhedra "commemorating" instances of "now" -- past and future? |
Beyond the simple movement represented above, a fuller animation is reminiscent of a flight of birds. Associating the polyhedra in this way is also reminiscent of Buddhist and Hindu metaphors of Indra's Net (or necklace) and its interest from a mathematical perspective as the continuous limit sets generated when pairs of generating circles touch (David Mumford, et al, Indra's Pearls: The Vision of Felix Klein, 2002).
Toroidal framings: The Szilassi polyhedron is presented above as offering unusual possibilities -- if only in terms of mapping. A previous exercise explored the use of another toroidal polyhedron, namely the drilled truncated cube (another of the Stewart toroids). This is unique in having 64 edges, suggesting the possibility of mapping the much-studied 64 conditions of change encoded by the system of I Ching hexagrams (Enabling Wisdom Dynamically within Intertwined Tori: requisite resonance in global knowledge architecture, 2012). Of interest in that respect is whether these constitute challenges to decision-making -- typically of "which" -- and how these relate to both questions and answers. The sections in that exploration are:
It is intriguing that engagement with "wisdom" can be explored through the questions which are the primary characteristic of each of the 48 koans of The Gateless Gate. In that culture they can, alternatively, be explored through the nature of the answers which are a primary characteristic of decision-making in relation to the conditions indicated by the 64 hexagrams of the I Ching (Transformation Metaphors -- derived experimentally from the Chinese Book of Changes (I Ching) for sustainable dialogue, vision, conferencing, policy, network, community and lifestyle, 1997).
There is of course a degree of debatable ambiguity in this distinction raising the issue of the nature of question and answer in relation to wisdom. Potentially interesting is then the possibility of relating the mapping of 48 koans to a mapping of the 64 hexagrams of the I Ching (Relating configurative mappings of 64 I Ching conditions and 48 koans, 2012). This possibility suggests an extension of use of geometrical metaphors to integrate any polyhedral representation of questions (Geometry of Thinking for Sustainable Global Governance: cognitive implication of synergetics, 2009).
The names mapped into the image below derive from an extensive adaptation identifying the "causal loops" by which the conditions indicated by the hexagrams are traditionally related (as noted above).
Drilled truncated cube with I Ching hexagram names associated with its 64 edges (reproduced from Enabling Wisdom Dynamically within Intertwined Tori: requisite resonance in global knowledge architecture, 2012, discussing mapping onto that form) |
|
Paradoxical focus of now: As with the attribution of questions and question-pairs to the Szilassi polyhedron, the attributions to the polyhedral form are necessarily tentative. Of particular interest in the Szilassi case is any choice made for the unpaired question of the seven -- the only one of the seven with a degtree of symmetry in its own right. It should of course be stressed that "now" is understood in relation to the toroidal hole that the polyhedron frames. This suggests that the choice of any unbalanced question is part of the experiential dynamic. It may emerge as unique in the moment.
Whilst a sense of "now" is framed by the configuration of questions, continuing consideration is required regarding the dynamic implied by that configuration -- as suggested above with respect to mapping onto the football pattern. One possible clue is offered by a degree of correspondence with the six-fold circular structure of the benzene molecule so fundamental to organic life. This is renowned for being dependent for its integrity on resonance -- leading to its recognition as a so-called resonance hybrid, as discussed separate\ly (Patterns of Alternation: cycles of dissonance and resonance).
This is suggestive of the possibility that, from a cognitive perspective, the Szilassi configuration of questions might well be best understood as a resonance hybrd. This could be especially appropriate to the dynamic quality of the sense of "now" and "feeling alive". It could be consistent with transcending the conventional binary modality (Transcending Simplistic Binary Contractual Relationships, 2012). In that sense "now":
Memory aid: Provocatively -- and with the poetic licence appropriate to going down the "rabbit hole", and given the quest for mnemonic catalysts -- it may be appropriate to explore aesthetic twists, transformations and resonances between focal terms of this discussion through imaginative word play, consistent with the riddle by which the sense of "now" may be characterized. This is partially suggested by the essay of William S. Huff (Homonym, Homonym and Homonym, and Other Word Pairs, Symmetry: Culture and Science, 1992). The terms variously related through such devices might then include:
Mnemonic exercise in configuring thematic elements |
Suggestive associations and correspondences | |
|
|
Is "to know" to be recognized as the verb "to now" -- to make now? |
With respect to memory aids, there is a certain elegant appropriateness to the possibility of using the common football as a mapping surface. This is all the more so in that the game of football, and the attention it attracts worldwide, is especially emblematic of the experiential preoccupations with the feelng of "being alive" in the moment -- with the "now" which is the focus of this exploration. There is of course a degree of irony to its "global" form and the manner in which it is so desperately kicked around -- globally. Conversely, to have "won" in the process significantly enhances the sense of "feeling alive" -- whereas losers are readily framed as "dead".
Experiential sense of "now"? A major limitation in the above argument is the descriptive nature of the articulation of the existential experience of "now". This may be partially remedied through questioning in the moment:
These experiential indicators are rendered even more subtle through the question pairing discussed above -- and the experience of "now" with respect to any continuum, most obviously where-when (with its correspondence to space-time). Various metaphors may be used to frame the moment in which the questioning applies, and the potentially relevant skill sets:
The above indications can be considered as part of a more general set relating to movement -- potentially "down the rabbit hole" (Clues to Movement and Attitude Control, 2002; Navigating Alternative Conceptual Realities. 2002; Towards an Astrophysics of the Knowledge Universe: from astronautics to noonautics? 2006). With respect to a web-enhanced knowledge environment, further possibilities may be imagined (From Information Highways to Songlines of the Noosphere: global configuration of hypertext pathways as a prerequisite for meaningful collective transformation, 1996; Transforming Static Websites into Mobile "Wizdomes": enabling change through intertwining dynamic and configurative metaphors, 2007).
Is the Szilassi configuration suggestive of the cognitive organization of a "wizdome" as a form of vessel for the navigation of knowledge space -- or perhaps as a "magic carpet" -- as framed by the following animation? The alternative abbreviation -- as "whiz" -- suggests a complex of associations with WH-questions and the sense of "isness" which is a characteristic of "now" (The Isdom of the Wisdom Society: embodying time as the heartland of humanity, 2003).
Animation indicative of the "discovery" of a cognitive "magic carpet" emerging from a "wizdome"? |
Relevant to use of the "carpet" metaphor here is the attention given to carpet design by architect Christopher Alexander in the seventh in a series of books on design, order and pattern (Notes on the Synthesis of Form, 1964; A Pattern Language, 1977; A Foreshadowing of 21st Century Art: the color and geometry of very early Turkish carpets, 1993). Surprisingly his work has had considerable influence on computer programming language design -- the essence of system comprehension. As discussed separately (Magic Carpets as Psychoactive System Diagrams, 2010), Alexander explores "carpet geometry" in terms of a set of 15 "transformations" (Harmony-Seeking Computations: a science of non-classical dynamics based on the progressive evolution of the larger whole. International Journal for Unconventional Computing, 5, 2009). He emphasizes the importance of geometric adaptation in order to enable comprehension of a higher order, as discussed separately (Harmony-Comprehension and Wholeness-Engendering: eliciting psychosocial transformational principles from design, 2010) The potential implications can be taken further (In Quest of a Dynamic Pattern of Transformations: sensing the strange attractor of an emerging Rosetta Stone, 2012).
Cyclopean symbolism of "nowing"? The following images offer an interesting contrast in relation to the above argument. For global society, the Eye of Providence (notably figuring on the reverse of the Great Seal of the United States and on the one dollar bill), has implications for the condition of the world at this time. The left-hand image portrays a barren landscape dominated by an unfinished pyramid of 13 steps, topped by the Eye of Providence within a triangle.
That pyramid, with its detachable triangle, is reminiscent of the Pyramid Ship -- a "Cheops class warship" -- of the alien Goa'uld, featuring in the widely disseminated Stargate series. It also recalls the ambitions of dissociated elites to abandon "planet Earth" for gated communities, and for other parts of the galaxy. The visible "gap" is then clearly significant.
The image on the right, however, positions paradoxical cognitive functionality -- in the "now" -- within the eye of the toroidal form of the Szilassi polyhedron. People may themselves have the possibility of functioning as "stargates" (People as Stargates: an alternative perspective on human relationships in space-time, 1996; World Introversion through Paracycling: global potential for living sustainably "outside-inside", 2013)
Symbolic locus of the Eye of Providence? | |
On the one dollar bill | Within the Szilassi polyhedron |
Given the masonic symbolism typically associated with that of the Great Seal (left above), it is surprising to note that one of the easiest ways to remember the complex geometry of the Szilassi polyhedron is as a 3-dimensional version of the square and compasses (otherwise overlayed in 2 dimensions), so widely recognized as the symbol most identifiable with freemasonry. The space they enclose -- to which such significance is attached -- can be associated with the hole in polyhedron, especially since variants of the symbol include a broken circle within it (in the form of the letter G). The square and the compasses can be mapped onto the polyhedron in two ways, as indicated in the animations below. Whilst such projections are an aid to memory, they also encourage reflection -- as so ably demonstrated by the popular appeal of the Dan Brown novels (The Da Vinci Code, 2003; The Lost Symbol, 2009), and the array of questions of which they are a focus, challenging the frameworks of convention regarding the externalities of where-when and which-how..
Projection of the square and compasses of Freemasonry on the Szilassi polyhedron as a mnemonic aid | |
Variant A (animation around vertical axis) | Variant B (angled animation) |
Paradoxical geometry: Of interest in making this contrast is the nature of the "pyramid" as a geometrical form in relation to the Szilassi polyhedron. The pyramid is not the simplest of the polyhedra, a 4-sided tetrahedron, as might be assumed; with the base, it is 5-sided. As noted above, the tetrahedron and the Szilassi polyhedron are the only two known polyhedra in which each face shares an edge with each other face. This has symbolic implications for the organization of the incommensurable worldviews of global society -- and a justification for its display in Reconciliation Place (Canberra) with the Mobius strip (as noted above). It is recognized that the next more complex polyhedron with equivalent properties has 44 vertices and 66 edges -- but it is not known whether such a polyhedron exists. The drilled truncated cube, featured above, is indicative of related mapping possibilities of cognitive significance (32 vertices, 64 edges, 32 faces).
The core experiential challenge is how cognition and identity are enabled and sustained by such geometry. Of relevance to this are the explorations of Douglas Hofstadter (I Am a Strange Loop, 2007) with respect to the Mobius strip, and also with respect to the Klein bottle by Steven M. Rosen (Topologies of the Flesh, 2006). The potentially paradoxical collective implications have been discussed separately (Sustaining a Community of Strange Loops: comprehension and engagement through aesthetic ring transformation, 2010). On the assumption that insight is not "cyclopean", the eye metaphor may also be represented in the following image of a Mobius strip (as was presented there).
Christopher Alexander:
F. T. Arecchi. Complexity and emergence of meaning: toward a semiophysics. 2001 (Paper for a workshop on "Complexity and Emergence", Academie Internationale de Philosophie des Sciences, Bergamo) [text]
Ron Atkin:
Stafford Beer:
Michael Caley. Mindscapes: the epistemology of Magoroh Maruyama. Routledge, 1994
Roberto Casati and Achille C. Varzi. Holes and Other Superficialities. MIT Press, 1994
Lisa Lai Shen Cheng. On the Typology of WH-Questions. Garland Publishing, 1991 [text]
Barbara Citko and Martina Gracanin-Yukusek. Towards a New Typology of Coordinated WH-questions. Journal of Linguistics, 49, 2013, 1, pp 1-32 [abstract]
Terrence W. Deacon:
Edward de Bono:
Jared Diamond. Collapse: how societies choose to fail or survive. Viking Press, 2005
Magali Sanches Duran, Marcelo Adriano Amâncio and Sandra M. Aluísio. Assigning Wh-Questions to Verbal Arguments: annotation tools evaluation and corpus building. LREC, European Language Resources Association, 2010
Abraham Flexner. The Usefulness of Useless Knowledge. Harper's Magazine, October 1939, pp. 544-552 [text]
Tim Freke. How Long Is Now?: A Journey to Enlightenment...and Beyond. Hay House, 2009)
R. Buckminster Fuller with E. J. Applewhite:
Branko Grünbaum and Lajos Szilassi. Geometric Realizations of Special Toroidal Complexes. Contributions to Discrete Mathematics, 4, 2009, 1, pp. 21-39
Douglas Hofstadter:
Douglas Hofstadter and Emmanuel Sander. Surfaces and Essences: analogy as the fuel and fire of thinking. Basic Books, 2013
William S Huff. Homonym, Homonym and Homonym, and Other Word Pairs. Symmetry: Culture and Science, 3, 1992, 1. (Paper to the Second Symposium of the International Study of Symmetry, Hiroshima, 1992)
Matthew M. Hurley, Daniel C. Dennett, and Reginald B. Adams Jr. Inside Jokes: using humor to reverse engineer the mind. MIT Press, 2011 [summary]
Irene Koshik:
George Lakoff and Mark Johnson. Metaphors We Live By. University of Chicago Press, 1980
Hilary Lawson:
Stephen Marrin. Intelligence Analysis and Decision Making. In: P. Gill, S. Marrin and M. Phythian, Intelligence Theory: key questions and debates, Routledge. 2009, pp. 131-150 [abstract]
Magoroh Maruyama:
Guerino Mazzola. The Topos of Music: geometric logic of concepts, theory, and performance. Springer, 2002 [summary]
William F. McComas and Linda Abraham. Asking More Effective Questions. Rossier School of Education [text]
George Miller. The Magical Number Seven, Plus or Minus Two: some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 1956, 2, pp. 81-97
Marvin Minsky. Jokes and their Relation to the Cognitive Unconscious. In: L. M. Vaina and J. Hintikka (Eds.), Cognitive Constraints on Communication, Reidel, 1984 [text]
David Mumford, Caroline Series and David Wright. Indra's Pearls: The Vision of Felix Klein. Cambridge University Press, 2002 [summary]
Peeter Müürsepp. Semiophysics as a Theoretical Basis for Scientific Creativity, 1998 [text]
Gunter Nittbaur. Stafford Beer's Syntegration as a Renascence of the Ancient Greek Agora in Present-day Organizations. Journal of Universal Knowledge Management, 2005 [text]
Nuccio Ordine. L'Utilité de l'Inutile. Les Belles Lettres, 2014 [summary]
Jean Petitot (Ed.). Logos et théorie des catastrophes: à partir de l'oeuvre de Rene Thom. Patino, 1988
Jean Petitot:
Jean Petitot and B. Smith. New Foundations for Qualitative Physics. In: J. E. Tiles, G. J. McKee and G.C. Dean (Eds.). Evolving Knowledge in Natural Science and Artificial Intelligence, Pitman, 1991, pp. 231-249
N. W. Richards. What moves where when in which language? Dissertation, MIT, 1997
Steven M. Rosen:
Catherine Rudin. On Multiple Questions and Multiple WH-fronting. Natural Language and Linguistic Theory, 6, 1988, pp. 445-501 [text]
Michael C. Ruppert. Confronting Collapse: the crisis of energy and money in a post peak oil world. Chelsea Green Publishing, 2009
Douglas Rushkoff. Present Shock: when everything happens now. Current Hardcover, 2013
Jesse Russell and Ronald Cohn. Szilassi Polyhedron. Book on Demand, 2012
Joachim Sabel. Partial Wh-Movement and The Typology of Wh-Questions. In: Uli Lutz, Gereon Müller and Arnim von Stechow (Eds.), Wh-Scope Marking , John Benjamins, 2000, pp. 409-446 [text]
Marjorie Senechal. Shaping Space: exploring polyhedra in nature, art, and the geometrical imagination. Springer, 2013
Peter Senge, C. Otto Scharmer, Joseph Jaworski and Betty Sue Flowers:
Carlo H. Séquin and Jaron Lanier. Hyperseeing the Regular Hendecachoron. [text]
C. Namwali Serpel. Seven Modes of Uncertainty. Harvard University Press, 2014
Gabriela Soare. A Cross-linguistic Typology of Question Formation and the Antisymmetry Hypothesis. Generative Grammar in Geneva, 5, 2007, pp. 107-131 [text]
L. Springer, K. Willmes and E. Haag. Training in the use of WH-questions and Prepositions in Dialogues: a comparison of two different approaches in aphasia therapy. Aphasiology, 7, 1993, pp. 251-270.
Russell Standish. Theory of Nothing. BookSurge Australia, 2006 [text]
Marina Stoyanova:
Lambert M. Surhone, Mariam T. Tennoe and Susan F. Henssonow. Szilassi Polyhedron. VDM Publishing, 2010
Nassim Nicholas Taleb:
René Thom:
Dmitri Tymoczko:
Robert Webb. Stella: Polyhedron Navigator. Culture and Science, 11, 2000, 1-4, pp. 231-268 [text]
Maurice I. Yolles. Knowledge Cybernetics: a metaphor for post-normal science. In: Steve Wallis (Ed), Cybernetics and Systems Theory in Management: Tools, Views and Advancements, IGI Global, Hershey, 2010 [text]
Maurice Yolles, B.R. Frieden and G. Kemp. Toward a formal theory of socioculture: a yin-yang information-based theory of social change. Kybernetes, 37, 2008, 7, pp. 850 - 909 [abstract]
Maurice Yolles and Gerhard Fink. Exploring Mindset Agency Theory. Social Science Research Network, 2013 [text]
Maurice Yolles and Gerhard Fink:
Arthur M. Young:
For further updates on this site, subscribe here |